|
|
ricerca
nel sito motori di rierca
|
|
|
|
|
documenti nella sezione
|
|
|
acquisizione
del linguaggio nei bambini autistici: cosa ci puņ dire?
(cash j. r., 1989)
come aiutare
un bambino con disturbo dello spettro autistico a svolgere
i compiti a casa?
(mona a., 2001)
comprensione degli
studenti con sindrome di asperger, direttive per gli insegnanti
(williams k., 1995)
excursus
storico sulla comunicazione facilitata
(cenciarelli i., mona a., 1999)
il parent
training razionale-emotivo per genitori di bambini difficili
(di pietro m.)
il programma
teacch
(arduino g. m.)
il self-management
(edelson s. m.)
informazioni
base sull'auditory integration training
(edelson s. m.)
interventi
terapeutici: modello comportamentale
(cenciarelli i., mona a., 1999)
interventi
terapeutici: modello organicista
(cenciarelli i., mona a., 1999)
interventi
terapeutici: modello psicodinamico
(cenciarelli i., mona a., 1999)
interventi
terapeutici: modello sistemico-relazionale
(cenciarelli i., mona a., 1999)
l'eit:
analisi di due casi
(lucioni r., pervenuto alla bma il 23-06-2001)
la vitamina
c nella prevenzione e trattamento dell'autismo
(rimland b., 1999)
modelli
di musicoteraia per l'autismo
(cenciarelli, mona, de rubeis, botta, 2002)
musicoterapia e autismo
- abstract
(cremaschi trovesi g., 1999)
pecs,
pyramid approach of education
(dal sito www.pecs.com)
prevenzione
(gruppo di lavoro tecnico-scientifico sulla sindrome autistica
della regione lombardia, a cura di cenciarelli i., 1999)
progetto
iem
(guazzo g. m., aliperta d. pervenuto alla bma il 12-11-2000)
sindrome
dell'X fragile e autismo
(dagli atti del convegno scientifico internazionale, 1990;
a cura di cenciarelli i., 1999)
trattamenti nei
disturbi generalizzati dello sviluppo - abstract
(marando r.)
un approccio
musicoterapeutico alla sindrome autistica
(lubrano m. l., picconi c., polcaro f., pervenuto agli argonauti
il 29-11-2000)
|
|
|
indietro
| inizio
|
|
|
LANGUAGE ACQUISITION IN AUTISTIC CHILDREN: WHAT DOES IT TELL US?
The purpose of this paper is to gain theoretical insight into language
acquisition in humans by examining the characteristics of language
in the autistic child. There have been many theories put forth to
help us understand how language may be learned. Especially interesting
is the question raised by treatments designed by Lovass (1966) to
teach language to autistic children. Lovass employed a behaviour
modification (therapy) procedure utilizing reinforcement learning
theory and shaping techniques to develop a "program for the
establishment of speech in psychotic children" (Wing, 1966,
p.115). The main questions arising from his program are: Were the
children actually learning ie. acquiring language; that is, were
they acquiring the facility to extract linguistic rules in order
to produce sentences appropriate to situational changes? Or, were
the children merely learning (assembling) a highly complex "phrase
book" in which they had compiled a repertoire of sentences
for all occasions? Put simply, do these autistic children understand
their newly learned speech in a way that is at all similar to the
understanding of a normal child of equal linguistic development?
Let us first examine some characteristics of autistic language as
reported by Aug (1974). Then, after a brief overview of the Lovass
method of behaviour therapy, we will attempt to determine the effectiveness
of his therapy. Aug (1974) outlines nine characteristics of language
that are common in autistic children. Later we shall see how Lovass
applied reinforcement theory to modify some of these so that speech
can be learned. Aug begins by pointing out a failure in most autistic
children to communicate a suitable affect in their speech due to
an impairment of qualities such as intonation, pitch, emphasis and
timbre. He calls this sing-song or monotonous speech. He also found
that these children would repeatedly recite stereotyped words and
phrases in a ritualistic way, such as the slogan in a television
commercial, or a specific question that may be endlessly repeated
and asked of everyone. Yet, often was the case where autistic speech
failed to address anyone specifically. Even so, these children had
special difficulty with personal pronouns even when directing their
speech to another person. Especially problematic are the pronouns
"I" and "you". And if these pronouns were ever
acquired, they were misused. Aug indicates that "yes"
is often a difficult word for these children to use. Instead the
child who wishes to respond in the affirmative will repeat the question
that was just asked. This Aug calls affirmation by repetition. He
also found that these children would prompt the speech of the other
person in order to strictly adhere to the script of a previous conversation,
or the child may directly prescribe the other person to say something
by commanding, "Say _________". Autistic children also
typically employ extreme literalness in communication as illustrated
by the example of one child who said that a picture is not "on"
the wall but "next to" the wall. Idiosyncratic metaphorical
speech is another common characteristic in these children. Aug notes
an observation by Kanner in 1946 where a seven-year old boy said,
"Annette and Cecile make purple." This was an enigma until
the original situation was revealed to Kanner: the boy had five
bottles of paint which were named after the Dionne quintuplets--Annette
was blue and Cecile was red. The metaphors are rigidly bound to
the some initial situation which makes the metaphor's frame of reference
incoherent. Finally, echolalia is the term used to describe the
duplication of speech the child hears. For example, a child may
hear the teacher say, "What is this? ...cookie?" to which
that child will respond with same intonation and rhythm, "What
is this? ...cookie?". Echolalia is characteristic of (but not
restricted to) the speech of autistic children. Lovass understood
this to be a fundamental aspect of autistic speech and its abnormalities.
Aug (1974) concludes that "autistic children fail to use words
flexibly as vehicles of general meaning, and rather experience them
as undifferentiated parts of some unique original situation."
(p.165) This concept is elemental to understanding many of these
characteristics and the learning problems that disrupt language
acquisition in autistic children. It is interesting that Lovass
has entitled his 1966 paper "A Program for the Establishment
of Speech in Psychotic Children" rather than "A Program
for the Establishment of Language in Psychotic Children" (Wing,
1966, p.115). Perhaps he was aware of the formidable abyss that
may very well separate the two. Yet in the relatively brief description
of his program below, one can conclude that he has considered the
challenge and nature of the abyss very well. In brief, his program
is highly dependent upon the specific severity of the psychotic
child involved and the characteristics of that child's language
development. Initially, before any sort of speech reinforcement
therapy can begin, any and all psychotic behaviours that may interfere
with this program of language behaviour modification must be extinguished.
Echolalia is an important consideration because there is value to
any actual verbalization in this program and it was found that previously
autistic, echolalic children progressed at much faster rate than
previously autistic, mute children (the difference is between one
and a half years and eight months of training; in addition, the
previously echolalic children's language was higher quality across
the board). Yet, uncontrolled echolalia can and did interfere with
this program and it had to carefully managed. Violent self-destructive
acts common in more severe psychotic children obviously needed to
be suppressed before any attempt of communication or therapy can
be made. If the child is left unrestrained extinction of these destructive
acts does take place, however, it is slow and therefore very painful.
In one case, the child did not stop until he had given himself over
10,000 blows. Electric shock therapy contingent upon self- destructive
acts was immediately effective (although somewhat controversial).
After self-destructive and self-stimulating acts are suppressed,
a program of shaping can begin in which the process of acquiring
speech is broken down into many, very simple behaviours. The sequence
of behaviours begins and, bit by bit, each behaviour is learned.
Primary reinforcers (those that attend a physiological need, such
as food attends to hunger) are used initially because of their power
and effectiveness. First the child learns to be attentive: eye to
face contact is prompted and reinforced. In the use of prompts,
given a certain stimulus situation, one wants a particular response
to occur in its presence so that it can be reinforced. The prompt
cues the correct response prior to training, or with minimal training.
Later the prompt is fading so that the control of a response is
shifted from one stimulus to another. This method is used often
throughout the stages of this program. Next, the frequency of spontaneous
vocalizations must be increased. Therefore, any vocalization will
be reinforced. Incompatible responses must not be reinforced, however,
so attentiveness and vocalization must both occur together to be
reinforced. Then, single phonemes are vocalized by the teacher (beginning
with some vowels and bilabial stop consonants such as /b/ and then
progressing on to more difficult spirants) to which closer and closer
approximations are reinforced. Often, a manual prompt may be used
to form the lips into the correct shape, then slowly this prompt
is faded. The repertoire of sounds is slowly increased. Now words
can be formed by juxtaposing phonemes in the child's repertoire.
The data that generates the learning curves with these children
is accelerated positively. They are acquiring a discrimination in
which the response resembles its stimulus. These data are characteristic
of successful imitation training. Yet, these children still do not
know the meanings of the words they imitate. Next, Lovass begins
a program of language training to teach such meaningfulness. Lovass
explains (Wing, 1966, p.128) that practically all language training
following verbal imitation training can be understood as the establishment
of three basic discriminations: In the first discrimination, the
stimulus is nonverbal and the response is verbal. The stimuli can
be objects, symbols, behaviour, etc. A good example of this discrimination
is the labelling of objects or the description a situation. The
second discrimination has a verbal stimulus which could arise from
oneself or others; the response is nonverbal. When a child obeys
an order, for example, the discrimination indicates comprehension.
In the third discrimination, both the stimulus and response are
verbal. A conversation or question and answer discussions are examples
indicating this discrimination. Language training begins simply,
by training a labelling vocabulary. This training will involve the
acquisition of both the first and second discriminations outlined
above. It is argued that mastery of a particular label occurs when
it is generalized to all classes of that object. For example, when
a member of a class objects is presented, such as a chair, and it
is correctly labelled as a chair when it is first presented, then
the concept of "chair" is understood. Through the use
of all three discriminations, training can now extend into the following
areas in the order listed: appropriate use of and response to prepositions
and pronouns, preposition training, pronoun training (eg. I-mine,
you-yours, he-his ie. nominative and possessive cases), discrimination
of the personal case, combining discriminations of pronouns and
prepositions together (additive, more and more complex discriminations
must be made to a stimulus, there is also a savings across tasks).
Also, after three months time, there is shift from primary to secondary
(learned) reinforcers. After considerable time, these children were
trained to make relatively complex discriminations. Yet, Lovass
was concerned that these children were over trained--only RARELY
did they volunteer to speak. They seemed to be strongly responsively
oriented to the attending adult trainers. A program of training
spontaneous speech then begins. The program phases were overlapping:
the institution of demands, and the development of commentary and
story telling including the recall of past events. An example of
the latter: a story may be read by the teacher and then the child
is asked to retell it, or he may be prompted to recall it with questions
that reflect a comprehension of the story. Lovass also reports (Wing,
1966, p.140) that he obtained extra elaborations not expected in
the program design. These he cites as being good evidence that his
program was indeed having some effect on language development. He
uses the example of Ricky who once made a comment on growth during
the later phases of the program. During language training, Ricky
learned about the concept of size and many things that were large
and small were discussed. In one discussion, Ricky was told that
small plants grow to larger size by pouring water on them. Ricky
thought about that for a moment and then he said, "put some
water on my head". Lovass feels that distinctively human contributions
by these children, such as this one, defends his program against
objections that it would create "trained seal" or mechanical
qualities in the subjects. What insight, then, does this program
by Lovass give us into the concept of language and it acquisition?
A number of things must be remembered. We have seen in the Lavass
study a reverse of the process exhibited by normal children. That
is, in normal children, comprehension usually precedes production
of a form, and comprehension is usually the immediate state after
productive training. Also, from later reports of Lovass's treatment
(Lovass, 1973), one finds that the initial improvement in the children's
language has been quickly setback as soon as their environment failed
to maintain the same demands on their behaviour as the training
environment. Once the maintenance of the social interaction breaks
down, so too will much of an autistic child's learned language function.
The question remains, then, whether there can be a transferal of
language from a treatment situation to a natural one in which the
subject must "fend for him or herself". A context must
be created in which meaningful and necessary speech and language
must be used both in normal children and autistic children. And
serving as the ultimate reinforcer must be the effectiveness of
the child's communication in the child own environment.
References
Aug, Robert C. (1974)
The Language of the Autistic Child.
In E. W. Straus (Ed.), Language and Language Disturbances.
Pittsburgh: Duquesne University Press. (pp. 155-172)
Clark, H. H., Clark E. H. (1977).
Psychology and Language.
New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc.
de Villiers, J. G., de Villiers, P. A. (1978)
Language Acquisition.
Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Lovass, O. I. (1966)
A Program for the Establishment of Speech in Psychotic Children.
In J. K. Wing (Ed.), Early Childhood Autism.
New York: Pergamon Press.
Rosenhan, D. L., Seligman, M. E. P. (1989)
Abnormal Psychology.
New York: W. W. Norton and Company.
Straddon, J. E. R., Ettinger, R. H. (1989)
Learning: An Introduction to the Principles of Adaptive Behaviour.
San Diego: Harcourt Brace Janovich, Publishers.
Wing, J. K. (1966)
Early Childhood Autism.
New York: Pergamon Press.
|